By Diane Dean.
Tim Hughes once caught a plane from New York to LA. Sitting next to him was a woman, a striking brunette with big hair.
Mr Hughes helped her with her bags and then sat back to enjoy the attention of the unusually solicitous airline staff. Upon landing he wished her an enjoyable stay.
“I live here,” was her steely-eyed reply.
“It was only two weeks later, after I’d seen a magazine article about her, that I realised that the person I’d been sitting next to was Elizabeth Taylor,” recalls Mr Hughes.
The former IT professional has a condition called prosopagnosia, sometimes known as “face blindness”.
But people with prosopagnosia aren’t actually blind; they know when they see a face, but their brain can’t process who it belongs to.
An inability to tell the difference between two similar faces does not necessarily mean that a person has prosopagnosia, and difficulty perceiving differences is not the same as difficulty with remembering faces, though the result can be the same.
“One of the things that we think people [without prosopagnosia] do when they look at faces is to see the face more as a whole, rather than the individual parts that make up the face,” says Romina Palermo, associate professor of psychology at the University of Western Sydney.
“We know that people who see faces more as wholes tend to be better at recognising faces. We also know that people who have prosopagnosia don’t tend to look at a face as a whole as much, they tend to see the parts more often.”